Approach to musical rhythm in Primary Education. Perceptions of Chilean specialist teachers
DOI:
https://doi.org/10.7203/LEEME.51.25579Palabras clave:
Ritmo, Educación Musical, Profesorado de Música, Chile.Resumen
El aspecto rítmico es fundamental dentro de la clase de Educación Musical en la escuela. El objetivo de este artículo ha sido recoger las opiniones del profesorado de música chileno, en relación con el abordaje de elementos rítmicos -pulso isócrono, tempo, metro (acento métrico) y frases o patrones rítmicos- en tercer y cuarto año de Educación Primaria. Para este propósito, se realizaron dos grupos focales. Uno de los grupos estuvo constituido por 6 docentes iniciales (3-4 años de servicio) y el otro, fue conformado por 5 docentes avanzados o expertos (más de 12 años de servicio). Los resultados muestran que los medios utilizados con mayor frecuencia son el movimiento corporal y el lenguaje verbal, los cuales se trabajan considerando principalmente audiciones relacionadas con las preferencias musicales y experiencias previas del alumnado. Hacia el futuro, resulta importante potenciar los elementos señalados para la enseñanza musical y continuar analizando la praxis del profesorado de música en relación con la enseñanza del ritmo en el aula.
Citas
Almeida, M. (2016). Escolhendo o repertório coral: uma tarefia de regentes? Revista Música Hodie, 16(2), 25-34. https://doi.org/10.5216/mh.v16i2.45212
Arriaga-Sanz, C., Riaño-Galán, M.E., Cabedo-Mas, A., & Berbel-Gómez, N. (2017). Songs are taught, songs are learnt: musical preferences in early childhood. Music Education Research, 19(3), 309-326. https://doi.org/10.1080/14613808.2016.1214694
Álamos-Gómez, J., & Tejada, J. (2020). Interrelaciones entre acción y cognición. Aportaciones de la neurociencia a la educación rítmico-musical. Opus, 26(2), 1-21. https://doi.org/10.250504/opus2020b2606
Álamos, J., & Pérez, M. (2015). Paralelos cognitivos entre música y lengua materna en estudiantes de Educación Primaria. Un estudio sobre la influencia del lenguaje verbal y de las características melódicas específicas en la memoria musical a corto plazo. Revista Electrónica de LEEME, 35, 1-27. https://ojs.uv.es/index.php/LEEME/article/view/9866
Barrett, M.S. (2013). Vidas sonoras en y a través de la música: una investigación narrativa de la participación musical "cotidiana" de un niño pequeño. In M. Díaz y A. Giráldez (Eds.), Investigación cualitativa en educación musical (pp.117-141). Graó.
Bowling, D.L., Hoeschele, M., Gill, K.Z., & Fitch, W.T. (2017). The nature and nurture of musical consonance. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 35(1), 118-121. https://doi:10.1525/mp.2017.35.1.118
Burger, B., London, J., Thompson, M., & Toiviainen, P. (2018). Synchronization to metrical levels in music depends on low-frequency spectral components and tempo. Psychological Research, 82(6), 1195-1211. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-017-0894-2
Chemin, B., Mouraux, A., & Nozaradan, S. (2014). Body movement selectively shapes the neural representation of musical rhythms. Psychological Science, 25(12), 2147-2159. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614551161
Chen, J.L., Penhune, V.B., & Zatorre, R.J. (2008). Moving on time: Brain network for auditory-motor synchronization is modulated by rhythm complexity and musical training. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(2), 226-239. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2008.20018
Dalby, B. (2005). Toward an effective pedagogy for teaching rhythm: Gordon and beyond. Music Educators Journal, 92(1), 54-60. https://doi.org/10.2307/3400228
de Vries, P. (2010). What we want: the music preferences of upper primary school students and the ways they engage with music. Australian Journal of Music Education, 1, 3-16. https://search.informit.org/doi/abs/10.3316/informit.691041986666884
Drake, C. (1998). Psychological processes involved in the temporal organization of complex auditory sequences: Universal and acquired processes. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 16(1), 11-26. https://doi.org/10.2307/40285774
Drake, C., & Bertrand, D. (2001). The quest for universals in temporal processing in music. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 930(1), 17-27. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2001.tb05722.x
Droe, K. (2006). Music Preference and Music Education: A Review of Literature. Update: Applications of Research in Music Education, 24(2), 23-32. https://doi.org/10.1177/87551233060240020103
Durá, J.A., & Tejada, J. (2021). Rhythm pattern discrimination by primary school students. Research Studies in Music Education, 43(3), 528-547. https://doi.org/10.1177/1321103X19869056
Faure, A., Gustems, J., & Navarro, M. (2020). Producción musical y mercado discográfico: homogeneización entre adolescentes y reto para la educación. Revista Electrónica de LEEME, 45, 69-87. https://doi.org/10.7203/LEEME.45.16625
Faure-Carvallo, A., Gustems-Carnicer, J. & Guaus, E. (2022). Music education in the digital age: Challenges associated with sound homogenization in music aimed at adolescents. International Journal of Music Education, 40(4), 598-612. https://doi.org/10.1177/02557614221084315
Fitch, W.T. (2017). Cultural evolution: Lab-cultured musical universals. Nature Human Behaviour, 1(1), 0018. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-016-0018
Fiveash, A., & Pammer, K. (2014). Music and language: Do they draw on similar syntactic working memory resources? Psychology of Music, 42(2), 190-209. https://doi.org/10.1177/0305735612463949
Flaugnacco, E., Lopez, L., Terribili, C., Zoia, S., Buda, S., Tilli, S., ..., & Schön, D. (2014). Rhythm perception and production predict reading abilities in developmental dyslexia. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 392. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00392
Friston, K. (2005). A theory of cortical responses. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 360(1456), 815-836. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2005.1622
Fujii, S., & Wan, C.Y. (2014). The role of rhythm in speech and language rehabilitation: The SEP hypothesis. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 777. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00777
Gerry, D.W., Faux, A.L., & Trainor, L.J. (2010). Effects of kindermusik training on infants’ rhythmic enculturation. Developmental Science, 13(3), 545-551. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00912.x
Godoy, M. (2021). Criterios pedagógico-musicales que aplican directoras de coros para elegir repertorio en los coros infantiles. Revista Electrónica de LEEME, 48, 79-93. https://doi.org/10.7203/LEEME.48.20979
Gordon, E. (2012). Learning sequences in music: A contemporary music learning theory. GIA Publications.
Government of Chile. (1999). Law 19.628 on the protection of private life. Published on 28 August 1999. https://www.bcn.cl/leychile/navegar?idNorma=141599
Grahn, J.A., & Brett, M. (2007). Rhythm and beat perception in motor areas of the brain. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(5), 893-906. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2007.19.5.893
Grahn, J.A., & Rowe, J.B. (2009). Feeling the beat: Premotor and striatal interactions in musicians and nonmusicians during beat perception. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(23), 7540-7548. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2018-08.2009
Haumann, N.T., Vuust, P., Bertelsen, F., & Garza-Villarreal, E.A. (2018). Influence of musical enculturation on brain responses to metric deviants. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 12, 218. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2018.00218
Herrera, L., Cremades, R., & Lorenzo, O. (2010). Preferencias musicales de los estudiantes de Educación Secundaria Obligatoria: influencia de la educación formal e informal. Cultura y Educación, 22(1), 37-51. https://doi.org/10.1174/113564010790935222
Holliman, A.J., Wood, C., & Sheehy, K. (2010). The contribution of sensitivity to speech rhythm and non‐speech rhythm to early reading development. Educational Psychology, 30(3), 247-267. https://doi.org/10.1080/01443410903560922
Jackendoff, R. (2009). Parallels and nonparallels between language and music. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 26(3), 195-204. https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2009.26.3.195
Jackendoff, R., & Lerdahl, F. (2006). The capacity for music: What is it, and what's special about it? Cognition: International Journal of Cognitive Science, 100(1), 33-72. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2005.11.005
Jacoby, N., & McDermott, J. H. (2017). Integer ratio priors on musical rhythm revealed cross-culturally by iterated reproduction. Current Biology, 27(3), 359-370. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2016.12.031
Jung, H., Sontag, S., Park, Y.S., & Loui, P. (2015). Rhythmic effects of syntax processing in music and language. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1762. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01762
Kotz, S.A., Ravignani, A., & Fitch, W.T. (2018). The evolution of rhythm processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(10), 896-910. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2018.08.002
Large, E.W., & Jones, M.R. (1999). The dynamics of attending: How people track time- varying events. Psychological Review, 106(1), 119-159. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033- 295X.106.1.119
Levitin, D.J. (2006). This is your brain on music. Dutton.
Levitin, D.J., Grahn, J.A., & London, J. (2018). The psychology of music: Rhythm and movement. Annual Review of Psychology, 69(1), 51-75. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev- psych-122216-011740
London, J. (2012). Hearing in time. Oxford University Press.
Matthews, D.R., Ubbes, V.A., & Freysinger, V.J. (2016). A qualitative investigation of early childhood teachers’ experiences of rhythm as pedagogy. Journal of Early Childhood Research, 14(1), 3-17. https://doi.org/10.1177/1476718X1452374
Merchant, H., Grahn, J., Trainor, L., Rohrmeier, M., & Fitch, W.T. (2015). Finding the beat: A neural perspective across humans and non-human primates. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 370(1664), 20140093. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2014.0093
Modoran, M. (2014). Methodological approaches specific to rhythmical music education in the Romanian primary school. Procedia-Social and Behavioral Sciences, 116, 3160-3164. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sbspro.2014.01.726
Nozaradan, S. (2014). Exploring how musical rhythm entrains brain activity with electroencephalogram frequency-tagging. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 369(1658), 20130393. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2013.0393
Nozaradan, S., Peretz, I., & Mouraux, A. (2012). Selective neuronal entrainment to the beat and meter embedded in a musical rhythm. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 32(49), 17572-17581. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3203-12.2012
Orts, M., Pérez, M., & Tejada, J. (2014). Efectos de los modos de presentación de información en la exactitud de la producción rítmica de estudiantes de Educación Secundaria Obligatoria. Revista Electrónica de LEEME, 34, 36-55. https://ojs.uv.es/index.php/
Öztürk, A. (2010). Evaluation of the teachers’ views on the rhythm practices in musical activities in preschool institutions. Procedia-Social and Behavioral Sciences, 2(2), 3638-3645. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sbspro.2010.03.566
Patel, A. (2008). Music, language, and the brain. Oxford University Press.
Patel, A. (2009). Music and the brain: Three links to language. In S. Hallam, I. Cross y M. Thaut (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Music Psychology (pp.208-216). Oxford University Press.
Pérez-Moreno, J., & Reverté, L. (2019). Las actividades musicales preferidas de la voz de los propios niños y niñas de cuatro años. Un estudio exploratorio. Revista Electrónica de LEEME, 1(43), 19-34. https://doi.org/10.7203/LEEME.43.13985
Phillips-Silver, J., & Trainor, L.J. (2007). Hearing what the body feels: Auditory encoding of rhythmic movement. Cognition, 105(3), 533-546. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2006.11.006
Posedel, J., Emery, L., Souza, B., & Fountain, C. (2012). Pitch perception, working memory, and second-language phonological production. Psychology of Music, 40(4), 508-517. https://doi.org/10.1177/0305735611415145
Purwins, H., Grachten, M., Herrera, P., Hazan, A., Marxer, R., & Serra, X. (2008). Computational models of music perception and cognition II: Domain-specific music processing. Physics of Life Reviews, 5(3), 169-182. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.plrev.2008.03.005
Ravignani, A., Delgado, T., & Kirby, S. (2017). Musical evolution in the lab exhibits rhythmic universals. Nature Human Behaviour, 1(1), 0007. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-016-0007
Repp, B., London, J., & Keller, P. (2011). Perception–production relationships and phase correction in synchronization with two-interval rhythms. Psychological Research, 75(3), 227-242. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-010-0301-8
Sakai, K., Hikosaka, O., Miyauchi, S., Takino, R., Tamada, T., Iwata, N. K., & Nielsen, M. (1999). Neural representation of a rhythm depends on its interval ratio. Journal of Neuroscience, 19(22), 10074-10081. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-22-10074.1999
Slater, J.L., & Tate, M.C. (2018). Timing deficits in ADHD: insights from the neuroscience of musical rhythm. Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience, 12, 51. https://doi.org/10.3389/fncom.2018.00051
Snyder, B. (2000). Music and memory: An introduction. The MIT Press.
Springer, D. (2016). Teaching popular music: Investigating music educators’ perceptions and preparation. International Journal of Music Education, 34(4), 403-415. https://doi.org/10.1177/0255761415619068
Stupacher, J. (2019). The experience of flow during sensorimotor synchronization to musical rhythms. Musicae Scientiae, 23(3), 348-361. https://doi.org/10.1177/1029864919836720
Su, Y.H., & Pöppel, E. (2012). Body movement enhances the extraction of temporal structures in auditory sequences. Psychological Research, 76(3), 373-382. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-011-0346-3
Tafuri, J. (2006). ¿Se nace musical? Cómo promover las aptitudes musicales de los niños. Graó.
Teo, T. (2003). Relationship of selected musical characteristics and music preference. Visions of Research in Music Education, 3, 1-20. https://opencommons.uconn.edu/cgi/viewcontent
Toiviainen, P., Luck, G., & Thompson, M. (2010). Embodied meter: Hierarchical eigenmodes in music-induced movement. Music Perception, 28(1), 59-70. https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2010.28.1.59
Tomlinson, M. (2013). Literacy and Music in Early Childhood: Multimodal Learning and Design. SAGE Open, 3(3), 1-10. https://doi.org/10.1177/2158244013502498
Vicente-Nicolás, G., & Mac Ruairc, G. (2014). Music activities in primary school: Students' preferences in the Spanish region of Murcia. Music Education Research, 16(3), 290-306. https://doi.org/10.1080/14613808.2014.912261
Vuust, P., & Witek, M.A. (2014). Rhythmic complexity and predictive coding: A novel approach to modeling rhythm and meter perception in music. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1111. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01111
Wang, D. (2008). The Quantifying Analysis of Effectiveness of Music Learning Through the Dalcroze Musical Method. US-China Education Review, 5(9), 32-41. https://eric.ed.gov/?id=ED503006
Witek, M., Popescu, T., Clarke, E., Hansen, M., Konvalinka, I., Kringelbach, M.L., & Vuust, P. (2017). Syncopation affects free body-movement in musical groove. Experimental Brain Research, 235(4), 995-1005. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-016-4855-6
Yim, H.Y.B., Boo, Y.L., & Ebbeck, M. (2014). A Study of Children’s Musical Preference: A Data Mining Approach. Australian Journal of Teacher Education, 39(2), 21-34. https://doi.org/10.14221/ajte.2014v39n2.5
Descargas
Publicado
Número
Sección
Licencia
Aquellos autores/as que tengan publicaciones con esta revista, aceptan los términos siguientes:
a) Los autores/as conservarán sus derechos de autor y garantizarán a la revista el derecho de primera publicación de su obra, el cuál estará simultáneamente sujeto a la Licencia de reconocimiento de Creative Commons que permite a terceros compartir la obra siempre que se indique su autor y su primera publicación esta revista.
b) Los autores/as podrán adoptar otros acuerdos de licencia no exclusiva de distribución de la versión de la obra publicada (p. ej.: depositarla en un archivo telemático institucional o publicarla en un volumen monográfico) siempre que se indique la publicación inicial en esta revista.
c) Se permite y recomienda a los autores/as difundir su obra a través de Internet (p. ej.: en archivos telemáticos institucionales o en su página web) antes y durante el proceso de envío, lo cual puede producir intercambios interesantes y aumentar las citas de la obra publicada. (Véase El efecto del acceso abierto).
d) Se comprometen a actuar como revisores, en caso que así se lo solicite el Equipo Editorial de la revista.