Effects of modes of presentation of information on the accurateness of rhythm production by Middle School students
DOI:
https://doi.org/10.7203/LEEME.34.9863Keywords:
Perceptual Modalities, Rhythmic Perception and Production, Music Education.Abstract
Presentation of information in combined perceptual modalities could facilitate both the learning of complex concepts and procedures. There is an important amount of studies on this subject related to several school curriculum areas. Although, this is not the case in music education, despite the visual helps might have the potential to facilitate aural discrimination and memory by means of associative processes, as well as the enhancement of motor skills. This study have tried to discover if there is an effect of unimodal presentation mode (three exclusive modes) versus bimodal presentation mode (two crossed modes) in the reproduction of musical rhythm patterns. To do this, two experiments were carried out with Middle School children in Valencia, Spain. In the first experiment (N50; 21 boys; 29 girls; 14-16 years old) a contrast pretest-posttest design was adopted. Subjects were grouped in five conditions, each having information in one different mode: only static images (I) flashing the rhythm according to the proposed rhythm patterns; only sounds (A); only on-screen texts (T) that represented the rhythm pattern when reading; sounds and static images together (AV) and sounds and texts together (AT). In the second experiment a repeated measures design was adopted (N=10; 3 boys and 7 girls; 16- 18 years old). Each subject had to do the tests in all experimental conditions aforementioned. Some intervening variables were measured: previous rhythm skills, previous musical experiences, school achievement of last year, preferences on modes of information presentation and gender. Test tasks were to hear in turn 10 proposed rhythm patterns, memorized them, and imitate them by means of a computer key. The computer ran an ad hoc computer program that provided stimuli, data gathering and assessment of responses. Results of both experiments show statistical differences in favor of groups AV, AT (bimodal) and A (unimodal). It is suggested the advantage of presenting rhythm information with sound together with other means in the teaching and learning music processes.
References
Antovic, M. (2009). Musical metaphors in Serbian and Romani children: an empirical study. Metaphor and Symbol, 24, 184-202.
Brochard, R., Tassin, M., y Zagar, D. (2013). Got rhythm… for better and for worse. Cross-modal effects of auditory rhythm on visual word recognition. Cognition, 127(2), 214-219.
Brochard, R., Tassin, M., Baudouin, J. Y., y Zagar, D. (2014). How is Visual Recognition Entrained by Auditory Background Rhythms?. Procedia-Social and Behavioral Sciences, (126), 203.
Cassidy, J. y Geringer, J. (2001). Listening Maps: Undergraduate Students' Ability To Interpret Various Iconic Representations. Update: Applications of Research in Music Education,19(2), 15- 19.
Chandler, P. y Sweller, J. (1992). "The split-attention effect as a factor in the design of instruction". British Journal of Educational Psychology, 62, 233-246.
Colom, R. y Flores-Mendoza, C. (2001) Inteligencia y Memoria de Trabajo: La Relación Entre Factor G, Complejidad Cognitiva y Capacidad de Procesamiento.Gerais.Psicologia: Teoria e Pesquisa, 17(1), 37-47.
Dolloff, L. (1993) Das Schulwerk: a foundation for the cognitive, musical and artistic development of children. Toronto: Canadian Music Education Research Centre-University of Toronto.
Eitan, Z., Katz, A. y Shen, Y. (2010) Effects of pitch register, loudness and tempo on children’s use of metaphors for music. Poster presented at 11th. International Conference on Music Perception and Cognition (ICMPC11). Seattle.
Engel, A., Bangert, M., Horbank, D., Hijmans, B. , Wilkens, K., Keller, P. y Keysers, C. (2012). Learning piano melodies in visuo-motor or audio-motor training conditions and the neural correlates of their cross-modal transfer. NeuroImage, 63(2), 966-978.
Evans, K. K., y Treisman, A. (2010). Natural cross-modal mappings between visual and auditory features. Journal of Vision, (10) 1–12, doi:10.1167/10.1.6.
Frego, R. (1999). Effects of Aural and Visual Conditions on Response to Perceived Artistic: Tension in Music and Dance. Journal of Research in Music Education, 47(1), 31–43.
Goitre, R. (1972) Cantare leggendo. Milano: Suvini-Zerboni.
Gómez, S. y Tejada, J. (en prensa) Effect of visual dynamic representations on the discrimination and understanding of musical texture.
Gómez-Ramirez, M., Kelly, S. P., Molholm, S., Sehatpour, P., Schwartz, T. H., y Foxe, J. J. (2011). Oscillatory sensory selection mechanisms during intersensory attention to rhythmic auditory and visual inputs: a human electrocorticographic investigation. The Journal of Neuroscience, 31(50), 18556-18567.
Grahn, J., Henry, M. y McAuley, J. (2011). FMRI investigation of cross-modal interactions in beat perception: audition primes vision, but not vice versa. Neuroimage, 54(2) 1231-1243.
Johnson, C. (1991). Use of the continuous response digital interface in evaluation of auditory versus auditory and visual aspects of musical performance. Southeastern Journal of Music Education, 3, 97–108.
Kim, K. y Iwamiya, S. (2008). Formal congruency between telop patterns and sound effects. Music Perception, 25, 429-448.
Kirschner, P. (2002). Cognitive load theory: implications of cognitive load theory onthe design of learning. Learning and Instruction, 12(1), 1-10.
Levitin, D. J., MacLean, K., Mathews, M., Chu, L., y Jensen, E. (2000). The perception of cross- modal simultaneity. International Journal of Computing Anticipatory Systems, 323-329.
Lovelace, Ch, Stein, B. y Wallace, M.(2003). An irrelevant light enhances auditory detection in humans: A psychophysical analysis of multisensory integration in stimulus detection. Cognitive Brain Research, 17(2), 447-453.
Madsen, K. (2009) Effect of aural and visual presentation modes on Argentine and US musicians’ evaluations of conducting and choral performance. International Journal of Music Education, 27(1), 48-59.
Madsen,K. Clifford, K., Geringer., J. M. y Wagner, M. J. (2007) Context specificity in music perception of musicians. Psychology of Music, 35(3), 441-451.
Mangione, G. (1975) La Pedagogía della musica secondo Zóltan Kodály. London: Boosey y Kawkes.
Mayer, R., Heiser, J., y Lonn, S. (2001). Cognitive constraints on multimedia learning: when presenting more material results in less understanding. Journal of Educational Psychology, (93), 187-198.
Mayer, R. y Moreno, R. (1999) Aids to computer-based multimedia learning. Learning and Instruction, 12(1), 107–119.
Miller, L. M. y D'esposito, M. (2005). Perceptual fusion and stimulus coincidence in the cross- modal integration of speech. The Journal of neuroscience, (25) 5884-5893.
Ngiam, J., Khosla, A., Kim, M., Nam, J., Lee, H., y Ng, A. Y. (2011). Multimodal deep learning. Proceedings of the 28th International Conference on Machine Learning, (11) 689-696.
Orff, C. y Keetman, G. (1950-54). Das Schulwerk: Musik für Kinder. Mainz: Schott.orff
Schnotz, W. y Kürschner, C. (2007). A reconsideration of cognitive load theory. Educational Psychology Review, 19, 469–508. doi:10.1007/s10648-007-9053-4.
Sweller, J. (1994). Cognitive load theory, learning difficulty and instructional design. Learning and instruction, 4. 295-312.
Sweller, J. (2005). Implications of cognitive load theory for multimedia learning. In R. E. Mayer (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning (pp. 19-30). New York: Cambridge University Press
Serra, D. y Tejada, J. (en prensa) Effect of static and dynamic visual representations in aural discrimination tasks of tonal melodic patterns by school childrens.
Tejada, J., Gil, M. P. y Pérez, R. G. (2011). TACTUS: Didactic design and implementation of a pedagogically sound-based rhythm-training computer program. Journal of Music, Technology and Education, 3(2-3), 155-165. doi: 10.1386/jmte.3.2-3.155_1.
VanRullen, R., Zoefel, B. y Ilhan, B. (2014). On the cyclic nature of perception in vision versus audition. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 369 (1641), 20130214. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0214.
Downloads
Published
Issue
Section
License
Authors who have publications with this journal agree to the following terms:
a) Authors will retain their copyright and will grant the journal the right of first publication of their work, which will simultaneously be subject to the Creative Commons Attribution License that allows third parties to share the work as long as its author and first publication in this journal are indicated.
b) Authors may enter into other non-exclusive licensing arrangements for the distribution of the published version of the paper (e.g. depositing it in an institutional telematic archive or publishing it in a monographic volume) provided that initial publication in this journal is indicated.
c) Authors are allowed and encouraged to disseminate their work via the Internet (e.g. in institutional telematic archives or on their website) before and during the submission process, which may lead to interesting exchanges and increase citations of the published work.
d) They agree to act as reviewers, if requested by the journal's editorial team.